Neuroscientists at the University of Southern California have published a
paper, Neural correlates of maintaining one’s political
beliefs in the face of counterevidence (Scientific
Reports, 6, No. 39589, December 2016;
http://www.nature.com/articles/srep39589 ), describing brain responses
to evidence that contradicts personal political beliefs. Areas of the
brain that are activated by contrary evidence include the amygdala and
insular cortex. Those areas are associated with emotion,
decision-making, threat perception and feelings of anxiety.
When self-described political liberals were presented with evidence
that contradicted eight strongly held political beliefs, the amygdala
and insular cortex were more activated than when they were presented
with evidence that contradicted eight strongly held, but non-political
beliefs. When asked to rate their beliefs after seeing the contrary
evidence, people’s beliefs about the non-political topics decreased in
strength, but they didn’t significantly change the degree of their faith in their
political beliefs. The contrary evidence was five statements of fact
that contradicted each of the political and non-political beliefs.
According to the paper: “People often discount evidence that
contradicts their firmly held beliefs. However, little is known about
the neural mechanisms that govern this behavior. We used neuroimaging to
investigate the neural systems involved in maintaining belief in the
face of counterevidence, presenting 40 liberals with arguments that
contradicted their strongly held political and non-political views.
Challenges to political beliefs produced increased activity in the
default mode network—a set of interconnected structures associated with
self-representation and disengagement from the external world. . . . We
also found that participants who changed their minds more showed less
BOLD* signal [detectable brain activity] in the insula and the amygdala
when evaluating counterevidence. These results highlight the role of
emotion in belief-change resistance and offer insight into the neural
systems involved in belief maintenance, motivated reasoning, and related
phenomena.”
* BOLD: blood oxygen level dependent
The amygdala are the green areas in the brain scan
The
amygdala and insular cortex are brain areas associated with thinking
about personal identity and abstract or deep thinking that disengages
from present reality.
The paper puts the research into context:
“Few things are as fundamental to human progress as our ability to
arrive at a shared understanding of the world. The advancement of
science depends on this, as does the accumulation of cultural knowledge
in general. Every collaboration, whether in the solitude of a marriage
or in a formal alliance between nations, requires that the beliefs of
those involved remain open to mutual influence through conversation.
Data on any topic—from climate science to epidemiology—must first be
successfully communicated and <em>believed</em> before it
can inform personal behavior or public policy. Viewed in this light, the
inability to change another person’s mind through evidence and
argument, or to have one’s own mind changed in turn, stands out as a
problem of great societal importance. Both human knowledge and human
cooperation depend upon such feats of cognitive and emotional
flexibility.”
Other observations from the paper: “It is well
known that people often resist changing their beliefs when directly
challenged, especially when these beliefs are central to their identity.
In some cases, exposure to counterevidence may even increase a person’s
confidence that his or her cherished beliefs are true. . . . One model
of belief maintenance holds that when confronted with counterevidence,
people experience negative emotions borne of conflict between the
perceived importance of their existing beliefs and the uncertainty
created by the new information.”
The human mind very much
dislikes uncertainty. It is extremely adept at quickly and unconsciously
removing uncertainty via rationalization and just making stuff up until
uncertainty goes away.
The paper raises some obvious questions. Is an inability to change another person’s mind through evidence and
argument, or to have one’s own mind changed, a significant social problem? Is it more ethical or moral to
retain one’s core beliefs, even when faced with evidence that those
beliefs are factually wrong? In other words, is it better to stand on
ideological or moral principle, or, is cognitive and emotional
flexibility (pragmatism) a more ethical or moral mind set?
ScienceDaily also discusses this paper: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/12/161223115757.htm
Pragmatic politics focused on the public interest for those uncomfortable with America's two-party system and its way of doing politics. Considering the interface of politics with psychology, cognitive science, social behavior, morality and history.
Etiquette
DP Etiquette
First rule: Don't be a jackass.
Other rules: Do not attack or insult people you disagree with. Engage with facts, logic and beliefs. Out of respect for others, please provide some sources for the facts and truths you rely on if you are asked for that. If emotion is getting out of hand, get it back in hand. To limit dehumanizing people, don't call people or whole groups of people disrespectful names, e.g., stupid, dumb or liar. Insulting people is counterproductive to rational discussion. Insult makes people angry and defensive. All points of view are welcome, right, center, left and elsewhere. Just disagree, but don't be belligerent or reject inconvenient facts, truths or defensible reasoning.
Wednesday, December 28, 2016
Saturday, December 24, 2016
The awakening: The mainstream media and post truth politics
In the last 6 months or so, and
especially the last month, the number of references to social and cognitive
science in politics and politics-related mainstream media articles and
broadcasts seems to have skyrocketed. That’s based on the MSM sources mostly
relied on for information cited as truth here at B&B.[1] The reason behind
the new interest in the intersection between cognitive and social science with
politics is clearly being driven by the explosion of fake news and the rapid rise
of post truth politics that coincides with the rise of Donald Trump and American
populism.
If interest in the biological science
of politics is real, it’s arguably literally the best thing that has happened to
both American politics and the MSM since the founding of the Republic.
Obviously, there’s a pro-science bias behind that opinion. Maybe B&B is
seeing something that’s not there, but it sure is a convincing illusion.
Getting to the point: The clearest and most pointed and detailed acknowledgement
of the role of science in post truth politics comes from the December 22, 2016
broadcast of Warren Onley’s program To
The Point (the 51 minute podcast is here, Barbara Bogaev guest hosting for
Olney).
The program’s title is The year in (fake)
news. Some of the program’s comments and their location in the podcast are described
below.
The program is in 3 parts. The
first part is irrelevant. The second is the 32 minute core fake news broadcast (The way forward in a post-truth world) and
the last 10-minute segment, Talking point,
describes (i) some of the science behind the human mind, (ii) it’s hard wiring
to be irrational, and (iii) it’s susceptibility to fake news via social media.
0:25 to 1:00: The rise of post
truth politics coincides with the rise of the power and influence of social
media and its algorithms, which have ‘supercharged’ fake news and its potency. Fake
news has sometimes caused violence. It’s now nearly impossible for opposing
partisans to agree on facts. This new free speech technology represents a new threat
to democracy. The role of the MSM, technology companies and educators will have
in untangling the bogus from the real is unclear.
8:20-9:00: Fake news is as old as
“news itself.” What’s new is social media technology and the speed and potency
it confers on fake news. Ad sales and algorithms help spread false stories,
e.g., Trump won the popular vote.
9:35-10:48: Fake news needs to be
defined because it’s in the eye of the beholder. Despite a long fake news
pedigree, it’s now different in terms of its power and speed. The modern
version of fake news differs from old fake news by the difference in (i) it’s
degree of intensity and speed, (ii) it’s reality distorting and persuasive
power, partly due to its ability to present one plausible sounding partisan
view without a counterpoint.
10:54-12:22: Fake news is also driven
by the conflation of entertainment and news. People now have a hard
distinguishing news from entertainment. News and entertainment are now more or
less the same thing. Donald Trump is a natural result of the conflation.
13:19-13:50: One effect of fake
news is its power to portray a sustaining image of the goodness of your side
and the evil of the other side. The data indicates that the effect applies to
both sides but is more pronounced for the conservative side, led by Fox News,
than for the left, meaning that people on the right tend to be more susceptible
to fake news compared to liberals.
14:27-15:34: Cultural change is
also relevant. General distrust of the MSM has risen, especially on the right.
Since the 1950’s, conservatives have been accusing the MSM of liberal bias. Conservatives
now tend to evaluate or weigh news based on its ideology, not its objectivity. That
opens the door for fake news that fits their ideological beliefs.
15:57-16:40: Some people run fake
news web sites for ideological-political purposes and some do it for money (discussed previously).
16:48-18:00: Motivated reasoning
(a powerful unconscious fact and reason distorting bias, discussed previously),
generates (i) a susceptibility to believe what fits personal ideology and world
beliefs, and (ii) reject what doesn’t fit. That biology feeds into why people
go to and believe in the content that fake news sites generate, even if the belief
is factually wrong. This happens to both liberals and conservatives, but is more
prevalent among conservatives.
19:05-19:33: Media literacy means
being more critical and skeptical, but calibrating those responses via personal
judgment. [A point of frustration: Once again, no one has any answer to the
critical question of who to trust. Everyone keeps throwing that responsibility back
on the individual. That ask is both unreasonable and literally impossible for
most people. It’s not going to happen now, or probably ever.]
19:39-20:36: Some recent studies suggest
that students through college level have trouble with distinguishing fake from
real news stories, especially for things you really want to believe.
20:50-21:48: One danger of fake
news is that it effectively makes all news fake whether it’s fake or not. Fake news
is a real threat to all news organizations. One upside is that real news outlets
like the New York Times are seeing an uptick in subscriptions, which seems to
be a response to the rise of Trump and fake news.
The rest of the podcast continues
in this vein. Incredible as it may seem to some people, one topic touched on is
discussing why true facts matter and how easy it now is to find ‘facts’, real
or not, to support just about anything that a person wants to believe.
Footnote:
1. B&B’s most relied-on sources for
information: Wall Street Journal, New York Times, Washington Post, The
Economist, Bloomberg Businessweek, NPR, and NPR affiliate broadcasts, e.g.,
Warren Olney’s To The Point program
that’s broadcast by KCRW in Santa Monica.
Social science & mainstream politics
Central themes here at Dissident Politics include the
intractable irrationality and incoherence of politics and the lack of
impact by modern social and cognitive science on the situation. In the
wake of Donald Trump's shocking election, that just might be starting to
change. This is something worth following to see if it's temporary, like a cat coughing up a hairball, or if something new
in political thinking is beginning to coalesce into some meaningful mind
set change.
Writing in the Wall Street Journal on December 22, 2016, Hoover Institution senior fellow John H. Cochrane sounded like he was beginning to wake up to the reality of how democracy really works in the real world. The Hoover Institution is an influential, hard core conservative think tank at Stanford University. Cochrane, an economist, was refreshingly candid about his learning moments:
“I have learned some deep lessons from this election and especially its aftermath. Like most policy-wonk types I supposed that people care about policies, and about results, and vote accordingly. And are amenable to sensible discussion about policy, and sensible negotiation. . . . What has become very clear to me since the election is a fact probably blindingly obvious to real students of politics—that’s not at all how it works. Most people vote by cultural affinity, brand, values, and a sense of personal identity. To the extent policy matters at all, it’s part of the buzzwords, propaganda and tag lines thrown back and forth. These things are related to where you live and who you interact with on a regular basis, which is why geographic polarization is such a problem—and why measures like the electoral college, which push our democracy to have more even representation of tribal and partisan alignments and identities are so important.”
That’s an indication that what social and cognitive science, including political science, have been saying about politics and how it works is beginning to sink in with at least some members the punditocracy. Those folks are serious and principled about reality-based democracy and rationality.
Of course, pundits who are in it to win at all costs have been aware of the science for decades. They successfully used the knowledge to deceive and manipulate to get what they want. In 2016, that crass class of players finally got what they have been angling for. They have created the new world of post truth politics that’s now threatening American democracy and other Western liberal democracies. For that crowd, truth is irrelevant. Winning is everything. Deceit and lies are key ingredients.
What’s new is that some principled pundits and members of the mainstream media are beginning to wake up to the threat. A period of trial and error to figure out how to deal with the lies and deceit has started. Depending on which side of the truth you’re on, that’s either good or bad.
All of that raises some questions. Are we really in a new era of post truth politics, or has it always been post truth? Does the situation seem different only because of the rise of fake news sites and sources? Or, is it meaningfully different because social media and online propaganda sources have risen in influence? Or, is post truth politics nothing to be concerned with since lies and deceit in politics are constitutionally protected free speech?
Southern Steenbok
Writing in the Wall Street Journal on December 22, 2016, Hoover Institution senior fellow John H. Cochrane sounded like he was beginning to wake up to the reality of how democracy really works in the real world. The Hoover Institution is an influential, hard core conservative think tank at Stanford University. Cochrane, an economist, was refreshingly candid about his learning moments:
“I have learned some deep lessons from this election and especially its aftermath. Like most policy-wonk types I supposed that people care about policies, and about results, and vote accordingly. And are amenable to sensible discussion about policy, and sensible negotiation. . . . What has become very clear to me since the election is a fact probably blindingly obvious to real students of politics—that’s not at all how it works. Most people vote by cultural affinity, brand, values, and a sense of personal identity. To the extent policy matters at all, it’s part of the buzzwords, propaganda and tag lines thrown back and forth. These things are related to where you live and who you interact with on a regular basis, which is why geographic polarization is such a problem—and why measures like the electoral college, which push our democracy to have more even representation of tribal and partisan alignments and identities are so important.”
That’s an indication that what social and cognitive science, including political science, have been saying about politics and how it works is beginning to sink in with at least some members the punditocracy. Those folks are serious and principled about reality-based democracy and rationality.
Of course, pundits who are in it to win at all costs have been aware of the science for decades. They successfully used the knowledge to deceive and manipulate to get what they want. In 2016, that crass class of players finally got what they have been angling for. They have created the new world of post truth politics that’s now threatening American democracy and other Western liberal democracies. For that crowd, truth is irrelevant. Winning is everything. Deceit and lies are key ingredients.
What’s new is that some principled pundits and members of the mainstream media are beginning to wake up to the threat. A period of trial and error to figure out how to deal with the lies and deceit has started. Depending on which side of the truth you’re on, that’s either good or bad.
All of that raises some questions. Are we really in a new era of post truth politics, or has it always been post truth? Does the situation seem different only because of the rise of fake news sites and sources? Or, is it meaningfully different because social media and online propaganda sources have risen in influence? Or, is post truth politics nothing to be concerned with since lies and deceit in politics are constitutionally protected free speech?
Tuesday, December 20, 2016
Tyrants, the new cold war and opportunities lost
December 20, 2016
In a Wall Street Journal opinion
(December 17-18, pg. A13), former chess champion Garry Kasparov describes his
experiences as a citizen of the former Soviet Republic of Azerbaijan during the
period of Soviet Union disintegration in the early 1990’s. The fall occurred a
series of events that included the resignation of Soviet premier Mikhail
Gorbachev on December 25, 1991. Kasparov saw Gorbachev’s resignation as the
result of “a final attempt to keep the Communist state alive.” Kasparov was
optimistic that “the Soviet Union would be forced to liberalize socially and
economically to survive.” Kasparov was filled with optimism that change would
bring a better future for people of Russia and the former Republics.
Writing 25 years later, Kasparov laments the lost
opportunity with the rise of the new dictator, Vladimir Putin and his
intentional erosion of democracy and freedoms in Russia and the former
republics. He see an attitude change where “citizens of the free world don’t
much care about dictatorships anymore, or about the 2.7 billion people who
still live in them.” That attitude change contrasts with John F. Kennedy observation
in Berlin in 1963: “Freedom is indivisible, and when one man is enslaved, all
are not free.” He argues that Ronald Reagan’s warning that “freedom is never
more than one generation away from extinction” might be an understatement in
view of recent changes in Western democracies.
Kasparov argues that “Bill Clinton was making jokes with
Russia’s President Boris Yeltsin and it was time to party, not press the
advantage. . . . . Yet instead of using it to shape a new global framework to
protect and project the values of democracy and human rights—as Truman had done
immediately to put Stalin in check—the free world acted as though the fight had
been won once and for all. Even worse, we made the same mistake in Russia and
in many other newly independent states. We were so eager to embrace the bright
future that we failed to address our dark past. There were no truth
commissions, no lustration—the shining of light on past crimes and their
perpetrators—no accountability for decades of repression. Elections did nothing
to uproot the siloviki, the powerful
network of security and military officials. The offices and titles of the
ruling nomenklatura changed, but the
Soviet bureaucratic caste remained as power brokers with no accountability or
transparency.”
As Kasparov sees it, “the reforms in Russia enacted by a
dream team of national and foreign economists were piecemeal and easily
exploited by those with access to the levers of power. Instead of turning into
a free market, the Russian economy became a rigged auction that created an
elite of appointed billionaires and a population of resentful and confused
citizens who wondered why nothing had improved for them.”
That somewhat sounds similar to the opinion that many Americans
have about their own democracy (as discussed before).
In 2000, Putin took power with few “obstacles capable of
resisting his instinct to remake Russia in his own KGB image. He also found a
Russian public that felt betrayed by the promises of democracy and afraid of
the violence and corruption we saw all around us. Mr. Putin’s vulgar rhetoric
of security and national pride would have worn thin quickly had the price of
oil not begun to skyrocket in the new millennium. A rising cash flow enabled
him to negotiate a Faustian bargain with the Russian people: your freedoms in
return for stability. . . . . Outside Russia, at every turn, Europe and the
U.S. failed to provide the leadership the historic moment required.”
Compared to right-wing dictatorships transitioning to
democracy, Kasparov criticizes socialism and communism. “Left-wing regimes have
had a far harder time, as if socialism were an autoimmune virus that destroys a
society’s ability to defend itself from tyrants and demagogues.”
Given the state of politics in America and Western liberal
democracies, the autoimmune virus seems to have established an infection there as well. How
it plays out in liberal democracy hosts remains to be seen. Support for tyrants
and demagogues is on the rise. Regardless of how it plays out, the opportunity
the West had after the U.S.S.R.’s fall was squandered and is irretrievably lost.
Any new opportunity for peace and freedom in Russia and other countries ruled
by kleptocratic tyrants and demagogues looks to be at least two generations in
the future, assuming another opportunity ever comes along.
The West blew it’s chance. We are beginning to see the
ramifications of the failure of short sighted, distracted Western political
institutions and thinking.
Monday, December 19, 2016
The scope of presidential power
December 19, 2016
Since the election, what’s
been going on with conservatives and Trump populists is not clear.
Conservatives are talking about constitutional conventions and amendments. Given the lack of coherence, it’s hard to know what
populists, or at least Donald Trump, are talking about.
The tyrant usurper: A major complaint from conservatives has been that president Obama has
abused and unconstitutionally applied or expanded executive powers by
unilateral action. One reason for that is, as one observer puts it, Obama was
trying to “circumvent congressional inaction or opposition.” That raises the
narrow question of whether a president facing a hostile congress has a duty to
try to govern via executive action when congress is broken. It also raises the
broad question of what the constitutional scope of executive power actually is.
Maybe Obama wasn’t a usurper at all and had a duty to act in the face of an
AWOL congress.
Given the constitution’s lack
of detail on many matters, including the scope of executive power, the question
cannot be conclusively resolved. Opinions on the various sides will be dictated
mostly by social identity, personal ideology and a personal rational that
supports personal belief. There’s no surprise since that’s the basis for most
political beliefs and the historical record is almost always open to various
interpretations. Put another way, most political beliefs, including ones about
the scope of executive power, are mostly personal and subjective.
The case for the unconstitutional executive: Writing in the Wall Street Journal (December 17-18,
pages C1-C2), Jeffrey Rosen, President and CEO of the non-partisan National
Constitution Center and professor of law at George Washington University (Yale
law school graduate), argues that executive powers “have ballooned far beyondtheir constitutional bounds.” Coming from a bona
fide non-partisan constitutional scholar, that’s a striking claim.
Rosen’s essay, The Over-Inflated Presidency, argues
that the debate centers on whether constitutional executive powers are limited
to what the constitution explicitly authorizes. One interpretation, the
‘conservative’ view, is that executive power is limited to those explicitly
enumerated. The powers include power to command the armed forces, at least in
times of war, if not always, veto of congressional legislation, pardon for
certain offenses, power to convene congress to declare war and power to make executive
appointments and treaties with senate advice and consent.
Another interpretation, the
‘populist’ view as Rosen sees it, is that the president has the authority do
whatever the constitution doesn’t explicitly forbid. Rosen fears that president Trump will have this mind set.
A third view, one that Rosen
doesn’t mention, is a pragmatic, public service-focused view holding that although
executive power is flexible or ill-defined, it falls short of imposing tyranny
as described by some reasonably acceptable conception of presidential power, e.g., no unreasonable or
unnecessary infringement on (i) constitutional personal freedoms, (ii) state
powers and (iii) congressional powers. For the pragmatic view, the devil is in
the details, e.g., what’s the definition of unreasonable and unnecessary? Those
concepts have meanings that vary with the observer’s mind set.
Of course, the conservative
and populist views also have their own devils. The constitution does not say that
the president is limited to only enumerated powers, or that the executive can
do whatever the constitution doesn’t expressly forbid.
Rosen, a believer in the
conservative narrow scope vision of presidential power makes the following
observations. They illustrate the practical difficulty in attempts to
definitively define constitutional limits on executive power.
Rosen points out that since
the constitution doesn’t specify if the president has powers beyond what was
enumerated, “it fell to George Washington to fill in some of the
gaps—establishing, for example, the president’s power to recognize foreign
governments . . . .” That’s an explicit statement that executive powers include
at least some matters the constitution is silent about.
There’s nothing surprising
about the existence of “gaps” in the constitution because the constitution
would have to specify every possible act a president would need to undertake,
which is an impossible task. President Washington ran into the limits of
express powers regarding the constitutionality of chartering a national bank.
That led to the birth of the concept of constitutional flexibility and that
some powers are implied to exist because they are necessary and proper for the normal
functioning of government. The courts continue to recognize implied powers (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Implied_powers)
that are not explicitly named in the constitution.
Rosen argues that Franklin D.
Roosevelt “exercised extraordinary powers across many domains: detaining
Japanese Americans in California prison camps, trying and executing accused
Nazi saboteurs, disregarding U.S. neutrality by implementing the Lend-Lease
program and ultimately constructing the New Deal administrative state. He did
all of this, it should be noted, with the tacit or explicit approval of
Congress and the Supreme Court.”
That raises the question of
why neither congress nor the courts acted at the time of FDR and any time
thereafter to restrain executive actions that are far beyond constitutional
bounds. If some of what FDR did was blatantly unconstitutional, why do most or
all of his illegal actions still stand today? The situation makes no sense,
unless one assumes that what FDR did was arguably within the scope of executive
power.
Alternatively, one could
argue that once an illegal executive action has been taken and not timely
challenged, it becomes legal by default of the legislative and judicial
branches. There’s no constitutional or logical basis to believe the latter
option applies, so the former best explains the situation. Even today in 2016, congress
or the federal courts could repeal or strike down FDR’s illegal acts if they
were in fact illegal.
When congress is AWOL: We are in a time when congress doesn’t function properly.
Partisan disputes have displaced regular debate, compromise and legislating.
Maybe that will change when the new congress convenes in 2017. Maybe it won’t.
Regardless, president Obama had to work with a hostile congress that has not
functioned normally at least since republicans took control of the House after
the 2010 elections. Under the circumstances, should the scope of presidential
power be as the conservative view sees it even if congress is dysfunctional?
Two fundamental problems: The constitution could have included language specifying
that the president either had powers not otherwise reserved to the other
branches of government, the states or the people. It could have stated the
president had no powers beyond those enumerated. Instead, the constitution is simply
silent.
Given the long history and continued
judicial and congressional acceptance of some flexibility over the scope of
presidential powers, it’s reasonable to believe that the president has some powers
that are not enumerated and those powers can include ones far beyond the
constitutional bounds that some experts like Rosen see. If nothing else, that’s
how American politics have in fact operated under the constitution. That alone
should count for something.
A second problem is that
arguments for a scope of presidential power limited to the enumerated powers
plus some “gaps” are rarely or never accompanied by any vision of what needs to
be changed, how that would be done and what effects on the American people that
would likely impose. Rosen’s criticism is no
different. He gives no vision for how government would work differently and why
or how that would be better.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)